Some biological hypotheses are widely popular, so much so that we tend to forget their original lack of success. This is particularly true for hypotheses with catchy names. The ‘Ghost of competition past’ is part of the title of a paper by the great ecologist, JH Connell, one of the many losses of 2020 (Connell 1980). The hypothesis states that, even though we may not detect competition in current populations, their traits and distributions may be shaped by past competition events. Although this hypothesis has known a great success in the ecological literature, the original paper actually ends with “I will no longer be persuaded by such invoking of "the Ghost of Competition Past"”. Similarly, the hypothesis that mothers of herbivores choose host plants where their offspring will have a higher fitness was proposed by John Jaenike in 1978 (Jaenike 1978), and later coined the ‘mother knows best’ hypothesis. The hypothesis was readily questioned or dismissed: “Mother doesn't know best” (Courtney and Kibota 1990), or “Does mother know best?” (Valladares and Lawton 1991), but remains widely popular. It thus seems that catchy names (and the intuitive ideas behind them) have a heuristic value that is independent from the original persuasion in these ideas and the accumulation of evidence that followed it.
The paper by Castagneryol et al. (2021) analyses the preference-performance relationship in the box tree moth (BTM) Cydalima perspectalis, after defoliation of their host plant, the box tree, by conspecifics. It thus has bearings on the two previously mentioned hypotheses. Specifically, they created an artificial population of potted box trees in a greenhouse, in which 60 trees were infested with BTM third instar larvae, whereas 61 were left uninfested. One week later, these larvae were removed and another three weeks later, they released adult BTM females and recorded their host choice by counting egg clutches laid by these females on the plants. Finally, they evaluated the effect of previously infested vs uninfested plants on BTM performance by measuring the weight of third instar larvae that had emerged from those eggs.
This experimental design was adopted because BTM is a multivoltine species. When the second generation of BTM arrives, plants have been defoliated by the first generation and did not fully recover. Indeed, Castagneryol et al. (2021) found that larvae that developed on previously infested plants were much smaller than those developing on uninfested plants, and the same was true for the chrysalis that emerged from those larvae. This provides unequivocal evidence for the existence of a ghost of competition past in this system. However, the existence of this ghost still does not result in a change in the distribution of BTM, precisely because mothers do not know best: they lay as many eggs on plants previously infested than on uninfested plants.
The demonstration that the previous presence of a competitor affects the performance of this herbivore species confirms that ghosts exist. However, whether this entails that previous (interspecific) competition shapes species distributions, as originally meant, remains an open question. Species phenology may play an important role in exposing organisms to the ghost, as this time-lagged competition may have been often overlooked. It is also relevant to try to understand why mothers don’t care in this, and other systems. One possibility is that they will have few opportunities to effectively choose in the real world, due to limited dispersal or to all plants being previously infested.
References
Castagneyrol, B., Halder, I. van, Kadiri, Y., Schillé, L. and Jactel, H. (2021) Host-mediated, cross-generational intraspecific competition in a herbivore species. bioRxiv, 2020.07.30.228544, ver. 5 peer-reviewed and recommended by PCI Ecology. doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.30.228544
Connell, J. H. (1980). Diversity and the coevolution of competitors, or the ghost of competition past. Oikos, 131-138. doi: https://doi.org/10.2307/3544421
Courtney, S. P. and Kibota, T. T. (1990) in Insect-plant interactions (ed. Bernays, E.A.) 285-330.
Jaenike, J. (1978). On optimal oviposition behavior in phytophagous insects. Theoretical population biology, 14(3), 350-356. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0040-5809(78)90012-6
Valladares, G., and Lawton, J. H. (1991). Host-plant selection in the holly leaf-miner: does mother know best?. The Journal of Animal Ecology, 227-240. doi: https://doi.org/10.2307/5456
DOI or URL of the preprint: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.30.228544
Please see the attached document, section "Responses to reviewers' comments", subsection "Round 2".
Dear authors,
I think you made a great job at replying to the concerns of the referees and opted not to send them the manuscript again. I do have some very minor comments still that I think should be addressed in the version of this manuscript that will then be accepted. Congratulations!
- Lines 52-53: I would start general, stating that previous herbivory can affect the performance of subsequent herbivores. Then, when later on you refer to the effect via plant defences you mention the possibility of actually facilitating future herbivory. I think this part is important because it adds a bit of unexpected outcome to your story. Otherwise, the prediction of previous herbivory being detrimental is a bit too straightforward.
- Line 58: a hormonal
- Line 61: effect
- Line 62: space between interactions and bracket
- Line 66: previous herbivory can also affect the oviposition preference of herbivores that arrive later.
- Line 67: can discriminate between.
- Lines 77-85: incidentally, we have data showing a match between preference and performance in absence of competitors, but not in their presence (Godinho et al. “The distribution of herbivores between leaves matches their performance only in the absence of competitors” Ecol Evol 2020). This is just a note, I think it’s an interesting result that could be discussed in your paper, but you don’t need to do it…
- Lines 125-126: I would say that it is more parsimonious to simply remove those plants from the analyses. Can’t you do that?
- In lines 146 and 148, both sentences start with ‘In order to’, you can easily replace one of them by ‘To’.
- Figure 1: I actually agree with reviewer 2 that this fits better supplementary material, but you can also leave it, it’s a matter of taste. However, the legend should be a bit more serious…
- Line 161: were run.
- Line 164: remove one of the brackets.
- Lines 191-192: this sentence was not in the original version of the manuscript, and I have difficulties in reconciling it with the previous sentence. If egg clutches were found in more than 90% of the plants overall, how can they, per treatment, be found on circa 40% of them?
- Line 192. I don’t understand why this sentence starts with ‘however’ nor why you are stressing ‘at individual plant level’. Isn’t this just the variance around the average numbers presented in the previous sentence? Maybe I am missing something important here…
- Line 198: for the sake of consistency.
- Line 201: I would say “Herbivory had no effect” because in the second analysis it is not the treatment per se that you are analysing.
- Line 209: I think that what you mean is that ‘larval weight was not significantly affected by the interaction …”. Right? Please state this explicitly, I got a bit confused.
- Lines 220, 221: same here, I would merge the two sentences: There was a significant, negative relationship between the number of egg clutches on a box tree and the subsequent chrysalis weight, which was not significantly affected by the interaction between the herbivory treatment and the number of egg clutches (Table 2, Figure 3C).
- Lines 227-230: please rephrase this sentence to clarify that you are proposing two explanations, not just one.
- Line 231: later, not latter.
- Lines 240-242: why? Can larvae move among trees? If not, I don’t think this is a proper explanation.
- Lines 242-243: so what? This may be interesting, but you need to spell out your reasoning here. Do you mean that it may be more important to accumulate those alkaloids for the future reproductive success of those larvae than to be of a particular weight? This may be true, but still, they are not facing the choice of no alkaloids and big vs alkaloids and small, right? Or is there evidence that pre-attacked plants have more alkaloids than clean plants? This whole issue needs to be further developed or excluded…
- Line 248: add a coma after hosts.
- Line 263: I would remove the ‘however’.
- Line 268: the ‘interference competition’ comes a bit out of the blue here. Is there any evidence for this in your system? And for the possibility of it being stronger in defoliated plants?
- Lines 270-271: I would write this statement in the same style as the others, ie: The number of egg clutches laid by BTM female moths correlated negatively with subsequent growth of BTM larvae.
- Line 280: was greater than.
- Line 282: I would state ‘this suggests’ instead of ‘indicates’.
- Lines 285-287: I would remove this because you already state this in the previous paragraph.
- Line 296: I would replace where by when.
- Lines 297-298: I would state instead: These cross-generational effects may thus lead to an important role of density dependence population growth.
- Line 307: add a comma after hosts.
- I find the conclusion still a bit too much attached to the system. I was wondering whether this can be linked to the Ghost of competition past. I will think about it a bit more and maybe write something on it in my recommendation, but maybe you can tell me what you think about this link before that…
DOI or URL of the preprint: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.30.228544
Version of the preprint: v2
The response below can be found at the end of the manuscript.
Dear Dr Magalhães,
We would like to thank you for your constructive and helpful comments. We revised the original manuscript accordingly. Significant changes in the manuscript are highlighted in bold characters. You may find our responses to your comments and to the two reviewers below, highlighted with bold characters. Wherever only minor changes were requested, we simply indicated "[R] --- Done", otherwise, we justified what we changed, or not, in the manuscript.
We hope that the revised version of our manuscript has addressed every concerns and will be suitable for recommendation.
Best regards,
Bastien Castagneyrol, on behalf of co-authors.
Dear authors,
First of all, I deeply apologize for having taken so long to comment on this manuscript. I hope that the quality of the reviews compensates for this long wait... I found this article interesting and straightforward. I particularly appreciated the scale and nature of the experiment, being an intermediate between a lab and a field experiment. The thorough and insightful comments of the two reviewers also point in the same direction: they both enjoyed the manuscript very much. They do, however, suggest a number of changes that I think should be addressed in the revised version of the manuscript. In particular, they both suggest including more variables in your analysis, which they (and I) believe you can do based on the data you already have. If that is the case, I think it is a cost-effective means to make your article more complete.
[Response] --- We followed recommendations made by the two reviewers and yourself and provide below detailed answers wherever necessary
Below I place my own comments, and I would be happy to look at a revised version of this paper soon. I only have two relatively major and a lot of minor comments.
[R] --- We changed the order of the two paragraphs as suggested and modified several sentences to improve the flow. Please note that we did not highlight every single change in the manuscript, only the most important additions.
[R] --- We now discuss this question:
Our findings may have profound consequences on BTM population dynamics. In most of Lepidoptera species, all the eggs are present in the ovarioles as the adult molt and larva body mass is proportional to fecundity (i.e., 'capital breeders', [@awmack2002; @honek1993]). As a consequence, host plant quality during larval growth and development is the key determinant of individuals fitness [@awmack2002]. Although the relationship between plant quality and herbivore fitness may vary among species [@awmack2002; @moreau2006; @colasurdo2009], we speculate that herbivory by the first BTM larva generation reduces the fitness of the second BTM generation, and that this effect may be further strengthened where high population density increase intra-specific cross-generational competition [@tammaru1996].
[R] --- Done
[R] --- Done
[R] --- Changed
[R] --- Done
[R] --- Done
[R] --- Thank you for these references.
[R] --- We restructured this paragraph, adding a new subsection ("natural history") and clarifying the description of first and second BTM generations (section "biological material").
[R] --- Done
[R] --- Done
[R] --- We screened every single plant and counted the number of egg clutches on all plants. We added the information on the presence/absence of eggs to the 'results' section.
[R] --- We do agree this is very unfortunate we have not been satisfyingly efficient in preventing larvae from moving among plants. Although we have been reluctant to present this data in first instance, we now reinjected them back into the manuscript, with words of caution in the discussion. Changes can be seen throughout the text in the "methods / analyses", "results" and "discussion" sections.
[R] --- Done. Indeed, it reads smoother.
[R] --- Yes! thank you for mentioning this. We have added this obvious argument.
[R] ---Done.
[R] --- This paragraph, starting with "Prior box tree defoliation by the spring generation of BTM larvae reduced the performance of the next generation" is about herbivore growth. We dealt with possible absence of cues in the previous paragraph.
[R] --- We removed "trait-mediated". Although we acknowledge that this would have been a powerful way to further address competition, we did not precisely count the number of larvae per plant or per shoot, mostly to avoid disturbance.
[R] --- Done:
the BTM is thought to have broad tolerance to variability in host traits, as suggested by previous observations that BTM larva growth rate did not differ significantly among box-tree varieties [@leuthardt2013a]. It is unknown whether herbivory induced changes in host traits are of the same order of magnitude as trait variability among varieties. However, assuming variability among varieties is greater, this result goes against the view that reduced performance of larvae of the summer generation resulted from box tree response to prior herbivory
[R] --- We deleted the mention to cannibalism, because this is true we do not have hard data ta back it up.
[R] --- We completely modified the corresponding paragraph to account for the several comments on the results and discussion.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
Reviewed by Inês Fragata, 2020-09-08 23:55
In this manuscript the authors test whether female choice for oviposition impacts intraspecific competition across generations. In order to do this, the authors compare oviposition and larvae weight of box tree moths on box trees previously exposed to conspecific herbivores or un-attacked controls. They observe that previous herbivory does not affect where female choose to lay eggs, but it affects larvae weight. This suggests that there is a mismatch between female choice and larvae performance, which is against the preference-performance hypothesis. The question that the authors are trying to answer is very interesting and can help us to understand better how species avoid intra and interspecific competition, even across different generations. Unfortunately, the methodological problems with the chrysalids made it more difficult to fully explore the potential of the question and experimental design. I have some questions/suggestions that may allow to explore better the data set that you have here, and go a bit deeper into your questions.
[R] --- Thank you for comments.
Questions/suggestions:
[R] --- We have now added this information and re-ran models accordingly. We therefore made appropriate changes in the methods | Experimental design and Results sections (but did not list all changes here). The reason we did not consider herbivory data in first instance was that potted plants were initially used in a completely separate experiment, for another purpose. We could not match every pot tags between the two projects, leading to missing data. However, because we agree that testing BTM response to actual herbivory rather than to a qualitative attacked/non-attacked factor, we decided to bring original data back into the main text.
[R] --- We agree that this would have been a great addition to the paper. However, we only counted and measured a subset of larvae and chrysalis (up to five), and therefore are not able to follow this advice
[R] --- Please see our response to Dr Magalhães, above*
[R] --- We released moths at the four corners of the experiment to reduce the risk of spatial aggregation (information now added to the manuscript). We have no data to support any explanation regarding the aggregation of eggs in one particular part of the experiment. This could actually be because of light (the part of the greenhouse received more sunlight in the afternoon) or because of fresher air arriving from the doors.
[R] --- We simply followed textbook recommendations when there is no a priori hypothesis on the shape of potential patterns.
we had no particular hypothesis regarding the shape of the spatial correlation structure. We therefore ran separate models with different spatial correlation structures (namely, exponential, Gaussian, spherical, linear and rational quadratic), and compared them based on their AIC [@zuur2009]
[R] --- Thank you for noticing, we corrected the text.
[R] --- Thank you for these suggestions. We adapted the manuscript accordingly. As for (1):
or that female BTM were indifferent to them at the time we conducted the experiment. [...] It remains however possible that BTM adults use other cues to select their hosts such as the presence of conspecific eggs, larvae or chrysalis.
as for (2):
it is also possible that induced defense reactions were delayed in box trees, or that they were already relaxed when we released BTM moths three weeks after the herbivory treatment [@karban2011], which remains to be evaluated.
Text comments
[R] --- Changed.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Changed.
[R] --- We replaced in particular by for instance.
[R] --- Done
[R] --- Done
[R] --- Done
[R] --- We preferred keeping this sentence unchanged to avoid repetition with the previous one.
[R] --- Done
[R] --- we clarified this point:
We initiated BTM larvae rearing with caterpillars collected in the wild in early spring 2019, corresponding to those that had overwintered**. We reared them at room temperature in
r 24 * 18 * 10
$cm^3$ plastic boxes, and fed them ad libitum, _with branches collected on box trees around the laboratory. We used the next generation larvae to induce herbivory on box tree plants (experimental treatment, see below) and the subsequent adults for the oviposition experiment.
[R] --- Done
[R] --- We have added this information to the revised version:
In addition, at this time, plants in the herbivory treatment had been cleared of caterpillars for three weeks (corresponding to the duration of the chrysalis stage) during which they were watered every two to three days from above.
[R] --- We have added this information to the revised version:
We kept them in Petri dishes without food for 24h to make larvae empty their gut and weighted them to the closest 10 µg.
We present variability in larval weight in the 'Results' section:
The mean weight of BTM larvae varied from
r round(1000 * min(d$L3.mean), 0)
tor round(1000 * max(d$L3.mean),0)
mg (mean $\pm$ SD:r round(1000 * mean(d$L3.mean),0)
$\pm$r round(1000 * sd(d$L3.mean),0)
mg).
[R] --- Changed
[R] --- We rephrased this sentence.
[R] --- Changed
We ran a generalized least square model (GLS) testing the effect of potted tree location in the experimental design (through their x and y coordinates, Figure 2) on the number of clutches per plant (*log*-transformed) from which we explored the associated variogram using the functions gls and Variogram in the nlme library.
[R] --- x and y coordinates referred to the design of the experiment (Figure 2). We referred to 'competing models' after Burnham & Anderson textbook (2002). We now state that the results would have been the same regardless of the spatial correlation structure. However, because this information is not essential, we preferred not reporting the detailed model outputs. Interested or skeptical readers will have access to raw data and codes and will be able to simple uncomment the corresponding lines of codes.
[R] --- We agree that this figure is not essential, but on the other hand we value this kind of illustration showing what the experiment looked like, because the reality is sometimes substantially different from what a 'Methods' section give to imagine.
[R] --- Pots were installed 40 cm apart. The numbers on axes were misleading. We removed them.
Reviewed by Raul Costa-Pereira, 2020-09-06 15:59
Castagneyrpol et al. present results from a well-designed experiment aiming to test the detrimental effects between conspecific individuals that have never meet. They studied a peculiar system where consumers (box tree moth larvae) have limited mobility, relying on their mother's oviposition decision to occupy good food patches (i.e., host plants). Interestingly, food patches are dynamic, and consumers can reduce resource quality to next-generation conspecifics by triggering defensive responses in host plants. Therefore, even if conspecific individuals never coexisted on the same individual host plant, the legacy of past "tenants" can reverberate negatively on current and future ones. Motivated by this interesting conceptual basis, the authors set up a greenhouse experiment to test how past herbivory affects preference (i.e., selection of oviposition sites by mothers, quantified as the number of egg clutches) and performance (i.e., individual consumer growth, quantified as average larvae body mass). Surprisingly, mothers did not avoid laying eggs on plants previously consumed by larvae, but larvae feeding on these plants with the legacy of past conspecifics were smaller. These results are exciting because they shed new light on the mechanisms shaping temporal dynamics of antagonistic interactions both between (plant-herbivore) and within (herbivore-herbivore) species.
[R] --- Thank you for this very nice summary and positive appreciation!
I enjoyed reading the manuscript and think it is well-written and the figures are well-presented. The experimental design is creative and statistical analyses are solid (I particularly appreciated how the authors accounted for the underlying spatial structure of their experiment in the models). Below I describe a few major points that came up while reading the manuscript, as well as some minor points that I believe can be helpful. Please let me know if you have any questions, I am happy to clarify.
[R] --- Thank you for your valuable and clear comments.
First, I feel that the conceptual framing of the manuscript is fascinating and could be contextualized and motivated more broadly in the introduction. Indeed, multivoltine insect herbivores are a great example of how individuals can affect conspecifics they have never met. Still, similar types of time-lagged interactions between conspecifics occur across diverse taxa (e.g., squirrels [Fisher et al. 2019 Ecology Letters], frogs [Pfennig & Pfennig 2020 Copeia]) and via different mechanisms (e.g., extend phenotypes, ecosystem engineers). Therefore, although the current structure of the introduction works well, I think that opening the manuscript with a more general view of ecological interactions among individuals separated in time would call the attention of a wider and more diverse readership. This approach could also help to reduce some overlap in ideas across the 1st and 2nd paragraphs of the introduction.
[R] --- We really appreciated this suggestion. We added a couple of opening sentences to broaden the scope of the paper
Biotic interactions are strong factors affecting the fitness of interacting individuals, even interactions are delayed in time and do not imply direct contact among individuals. Examples of such interactions can be found in both plants through plant-soil feedbacks [@putten2016] and in animals [@fisher2019; @pfenning2020]
Hypotheses and respective predictions could be more thoroughly presented to readers. The last paragraph of the introduction is concise and nicely describes the general hypotheses of the study (lines 84-85). However, readers will only find out how the authors investigated their hypotheses in the methods (e.g., lines 123-135), which creates a certain gap in the narrative flow. Thus, the authors could include their respective predictions as well at the end of the introduction (including 'operational variables' - e.g., We expect that plants that previously hosted larvae should [i] have fewer eggs and [ii] host smaller larvae). Moreover, as the experimental design allows inferring the contribution of purely spatial effects on oviposition patterns, the authors could at least mention this at the end of the introduction. By the way, I think that measuring and accounting for spatial structure in oviposition patterns is an exciting novelty of the manuscript. Thus, maybe the biological causes and implications of this spatial non-independence in mothers' oviposition choices could be more explored in the discussion.
[R] --- We have modified the end of the introduction in order to introduce "operational variables" as suggested. As for the discussion on spatial analyses, we agree that our results could pave the way for further investigation. However, the experiment was not designed to explore such spatial effects. We only aimed at controlling possible bias in the design. Therefore, we preferred not putting to much emphasis on this issue as it would have been very speculative.
Finally, I have a quick suggestion about a potential additional dimension of performance that could be considered. To quantify the effects of past herbivory on offspring performance, the authors compared the average body mass of larvae across treatments. I fully agree this is a key aspect of per capita offspring performance, and results are exciting in this regard. In light of the natural history of the system, as the authors also measured the number of L3 larvae per plant (lines 130-131), I wonder whether the conversion rate egg $\rightarrow$ L3 larva (e.g., number of eggs/number of L3 larvae) could not be used as an additional metric of performance. Is there evidence in the literature that past herbivory can affect egg eclosion rates and/or early larval development (L1 $\rightarrow$ L2 $\rightarrow$ L3)? This alternative metric would capture a different facet of offspring performance not necessarily correlated with mean larvae weight (e.g., larvae mortality associated with lower foraging rates and/or increased toxins).
[R] --- This would have been a great addition to the paper indeed. Unfortunately, we did not have such an information at hand for we counted up to five larvae per plant. The phrasing of the original version was ambiguous in this respect. We modified it accordingly.
Minor comments:
[R] --- We deleted the reference to multivoltine species.
[R] --- We deeply modified the introduction, please see our response to Dr Magalhães' comments.
[R] --- The corresponding sentence was deleted.
[R] --- We added "themselves" to make it clear that the female can detect herbivores, or herbivory-induced changes in plant traits.
[R] --- We rephrased this sentence.
[R] --- Yes it does! We now mention interference competition and resource depletion.
[R] --- We restructured the introduction to improve the reading.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- The sentence was moved to the "natural history" new paragraph.
[R] --- Done:
_ In order to confirm that the addition of BTM larvae caused herbivory, we visually estimated BTM herbivory as the percentage of leaves consumed by BTM larvae, looking at every branch on every plant. We then averaged herbivory at the plant level. In
r length(d$Herbivory[d$Herbivory > 0 & d$Herbivory_source == "Imputed"])
plants, herbivory data was missing and was imputed as the average of herbivory measured in other plants. In the herbivory treatment, the percentage of leaf area consumed by BTM larvae ranged fromr min(d$Herbivory[d$Herbivory >0])
tor max(d$Herbivory[d$Herbivory >0])
% and was on averager mean(d$Herbivory[d$Herbivory >0])
%._
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Yes, the duration of the pupal stage in the wild is long enough to have two separate generations.
[R] --- We deleted this sentence.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done: "only 6% of plants hosted less than five larvae".
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Thanks.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Yes, corresponding information is now available in Table 1.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- We added the following sentence:
However, we cannot exclude that some cues were mediated by larva frass, which was watched out from leaves when we watered plants.
[R] --- This is an interesting question that would be worth digging further. We are not aware of dedicated studies.
[R] --- "Trait mediated" was misleading. We deleted these two words and believe the sentence reads better now.
[R] --- yes, we have been quite surprised to observe this, but did not try to investigate it further. However,in the absence of back-up data, we eventually deleted reference to this possible phenomenon.
[R] --- This sentence was deleted.
[R] --- Changed to "within".
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
[R] --- Done.
I hope the authors find these comments helpful. Best wishes, Raul.
[R] --- We did, thanks!
Dear authors,
First of all, I deeply apologize for having taken so long to comment on this manuscript. I hope that the quality of the reviews compensates for this long wait… I found this article interesting and straightforward. I particularly appreciated the scale and nature of the experiment, being an intermediate between a lab and a field experiment. The thorough and insightful comments of the two reviewers also point in the same direction: they both enjoyed the manuscript very much. They do, however, suggest a number of changes that I think should be addressed in the revised version of the manuscript. In particular, they both suggest including more variables in your analysis, which they (and I) believe you can do based on the data you already have. If that is the case, I think it is a cost-effective means to make your article more complete.
Below I place my own comments, and I would be happy to look at a revised version of this paper soon. I only have two relatively major and a lot of minor comments.
In this manuscript the authors test whether female choice for oviposition impacts intraspecific competition across generations. In order to do this, the authors compare oviposition and larvae weight of box tree moths on box trees previously exposed to conspecific herbivores or un-attacked controls. They observe that previous herbivory does not affect where female choose to lay eggs, but it affects larvae weight. This suggests that there is a mismatch between female choice and larvae performance, which is against the preference-performance hypothesis.
The question that the authors are trying to answer is very interesting and can help us to understand better how species avoid intra and interspecific competition, even across different generations. Unfortunately, the methodological problems with the chrysalids made it more difficult to fully explore the potential of the question and experimental design. I have some questions/suggestions that may allow to explore better the data set that you have here, and go a bit deeper into your questions.
Questions/suggestions: - If I understood correctly, you have the percentage of damage per plant that was done by the first infesting larvae. You could use this as a covariate in your choice experiment to see if damage was a better way of predicting female choice, in addition to your spatial correlation structure. I think it could also be interesting to use the initial percentage of herbivory on the analysis of the larvae weight.
Besides the effect on choice of the egg laying females, herbivory could also affect plant quality and manifest in other life stages. In addition to looking at the weight of the caterpillars, since you have a measure of egg to caterpillar mortality, it would be interesting to see if egg to larvae mortality was higher in attacked or control trees.
Regarding the data on the chrysalids, why do you assume that it was the number of chrysalids that was wrong, and not the number of eggs or larvae (i.e. you could miss some larvae/eggs)? Does this excess occur more in non-damaged trees? Also, does the number of extra chrysalids match the number of missing caterpillars from nearby trees/attacked trees? Because it would be interesting to see if there was more dispersal for pre-attacked trees than for control trees. It is quite striking to have half your controls and 1 one attacked plant where this happens, so I wonder if there is something biologically interesting underlying this pattern.
However, if you are planning to not analyse the data or speculate on it, I think it would be better to remove the chrysalid part, as the usefulness of the information is unclear.
L148 - Did you release the moths in this region of the plots (between the 1 and 3)? because that could be a reason for the spatial structure to occur? alternatively did it had a source of light/heat or something alike? because it is rather strange that they clustered around that region.
L152 – Why did you use these different spatial correlation structures? and what does it mean to have these different spatial correlation structures? This is important to explain what are you accounting for in the analysis.
L163: 166 – From table 2 you have 3 models (and not two) that have similar performance, and they are not significantly better or worse compared to the quadratic one, as you need at least a difference of 2 in the AIC, using the rule of thumb from Burnham & Anderson 2004 (http://faculty.washington.edu/skalski/classes/QERM597/papers_xtra/Burnham%20and%20Anderson.pdf)
In the first section of the discussion, I think two hypotheses that you don’t mention are that 1) the moths may need cues from other life stages, such as female conspecific oviposition or the chrysalids; 2) you let too much time pass and the cues related with the conspecific were not present anymore.
Text comments L17:20 – This sentence is a bit weird, suggestion: “We tested this hypothesis in a choice experiment with box tree moth females (Cydalima perspectalis Walker, Lepidoptera: Crambidae). These females were exposed to box trees (Buxus sempervirens L., Buxaceae) previously defoliated by conspecific larvae earlier in the season.” L30 – remove the thus from “Insects may thus reduce” L32 – I think you mean assumes instead of supposes L38 – I don’t understand why you say “in particular” here, are those the only traits that will be important for females to detect that correlate with larval performance? for example presence of predators and competitors could be other factors that females may detect before ovipositing. L41 – “time-lagged consequences on the preference” L49 – I would substitute a mix of, with both L60 – I would replace the “later herbivores” by later arriving/appearing herbivores L61 – I would replace the “late coming herbivores” by later arriving/appearing herbivores L71:73 – This last sentence is not very clear. Maybe: “Thus, in order to quantify the effect of prior herbivory on subsequent herbivore performance, we need to assess how it affects both female choice and progeny performance in attacked and non-attacked hosts.” L99:100 – “Their adults...” whose adults? you mean that the larvae were used on the preference test and the adult stage on the performance test? maybe something like: “The adult stage of these larvae were used in...” L112 – feed on missing a space L117 –Any specific reason for waiting the three weeks? is it the amount of time that they would take to lay eggs again? L132 – Why did you wait 24h for weighting them? and not measured them right away? Also was there a high variance in larvae weight? L140 – 61 instead of 60 (or otherwise you have the number wrong above) L140:141 – maybe “and only 1 previously attacked plant” instead of “(and only in 1 previously attacked plant)” L146 – x and y coordinates of what? L166 – I would not call them competing models. Additionally, you should add whether they show similar results, since you cannot say which one is best. Fig1 – I would put this figure as supplementary material. Fig 2- I would like to know what are the x and y axis? meters? random unities?
Castagneyrpol et al. present results from a well-designed experiment aiming to test the detrimental effects between conspecific individuals that have never meet. They studied a peculiar system where consumers (box tree moth larvae) have limited mobility, relying on their mother's oviposition decision to occupy good food patches (i.e., host plants). Interestingly, food patches are dynamic, and consumers can reduce resource quality to next-generation conspecifics by triggering defensive responses in host plants. Therefore, even if conspecific individuals never coexisted on the same individual host plant, the legacy of past "tenants" can reverberate negatively on current and future ones. Motivated by this interesting conceptual basis, the authors set up a greenhouse experiment to test how past herbivory affects preference (i.e., selection of oviposition sites by mothers, quantified as the number of egg clutches) and performance (i.e., individual consumer growth, quantified as average larvae body mass). Surprisingly, mothers did not avoid laying eggs on plants previously consumed by larvae, but larvae feeding on these plants with the legacy of past conspecifics were smaller. These results are exciting because they shed new light on the mechanisms shaping temporal dynamics of antagonistic interactions both between (plant-herbivore) and within (herbivore-herbivore) species.
I enjoyed reading the manuscript and think it is well-written and the figures are well-presented. The experimental design is creative and statistical analyses are solid (I particularly appreciated how the authors accounted for the underlying spatial structure of their experiment in the models). Below I describe a few major points that came up while reading the manuscript, as well as some minor points that I believe can be helpful. Please let me know if you have any questions, I am happy to clarify.
First, I feel that the conceptual framing of the manuscript is fascinating and could be contextualized and motivated more broadly in the introduction. Indeed, multivoltine insect herbivores are a great example of how individuals can affect conspecifics they have never met. Still, similar types of time-lagged interactions between conspecifics occur across diverse taxa (e.g., squirrels [Fisher et al. 2019 Ecology Letters], frogs [Pfennig & Pfennig 2020 Copeia]) and via different mechanisms (e.g., extend phenotypes, ecosystem engineers). Therefore, although the current structure of the introduction works well, I think that opening the manuscript with a more general view of ecological interactions among individuals separated in time would call the attention of a wider and more diverse readership. This approach could also help to reduce some overlap in ideas across the 1st and 2nd paragraphs of the introduction.
Hypotheses and respective predictions could be more thoroughly presented to readers. The last paragraph of the introduction is concise and nicely describes the general hypotheses of the study (lines 84-85). However, readers will only find out how the authors investigated their hypotheses in the methods (e.g., lines 123-135), which creates a certain gap in the narrative flow. Thus, the authors could include their respective predictions as well at the end of the introduction (including 'operational variables' - e.g., We expect that plants that previously hosted larvae should [i] have fewer eggs and [ii] host smaller larvae). Moreover, as the experimental design allows inferring the contribution of purely spatial effects on oviposition patterns, the authors could at least mention this at the end of the introduction. By the way, I think that measuring and accounting for spatial structure in oviposition patterns is an exciting novelty of the manuscript. Thus, maybe the biological causes and implications of this spatial non-independence in mothers' oviposition choices could be more explored in the discussion.
Finally, I have a quick suggestion about a potential additional dimension of performance that could be considered. To quantify the effects of past herbivory on offspring performance, the authors compared the average body mass of larvae across treatments. I fully agree this is a key aspect of per capita offspring performance, and results are exciting in this regard. In light of the natural history of the system, as the authors also measured the number of L3 larvae per plant (lines 130-131), I wonder whether the conversion rate egg -> L3 larva (e.g., number of eggs/number of L3 larvae) could not be used as an additional metric of performance. Is there evidence in the literature that past herbivory can affect egg eclosion rates and/or early larval development (L1 -> L2 -> L3)? This alternative metric would capture a different facet of offspring performance not necessarily correlated with mean larvae weight (e.g., larvae mortality associated with lower foraging rates and/or increased toxins).
Minor comments:
Lines 1-2. The title is solid and general, but I am not sure if all readers will be familiar with the concept of 'multivoltine' (I guess it depends on the target journal).
Lines 39-40. Given the idea of the last sentence in this paragraph, it would be good to emphasize here that "competing herbivores" refers to different species of herbivores (i.e., interspecific competition). I would say the same about Line 42.
Lines 46-50. I feel that most of these ideas were already presented to readers in the previous paragraph. The argument presented in Line 50 sounds like an exciting way to begin this paragraph.
Line 50. I follow the meaning of 'passage of competitors' but it's possible that some readers may find it a bit confusing. One potential alternative (maybe not that accurate) would be something like 'the legacy' of past herbivores on host plants.
Line 51. Maybe the authors could provide a brief view of what 'direct' detection means in this context, e.g. "...mated females can directly detect (e.g., via visual or olfactory cues) the present...".
Line 61. Adding a 'triggering' or 'stimulating' before "defenses that generally" could make this idea clearer to readers.
Line 60. I am not an expert in plant-herbivore systems, but a first intuitive, simple mechanism seems to be the reduction of food biomass by previous consumers. Does it make sense?
Lines 65-66. This is very interesting, and the following example illustrates well this mismatch between selection by mothers vs. impacts on offspring. However, I feel a follow-up conceptual sentence would help readers to crystallize this idea by clarifying that not necessarily the effects on preference and performance are congruent.
Lines 74-75. Considering the broad readership of ecologists and evolutionary biologists this manuscript has the potential to reach, I recommend the authors to define the concept of 'multivoltine'. Not all readers may be familiar with it.
Line 75. Tiny detail: remove the italic from spp.
Lines 84-86. This is a matter of writing style, but one possibility here is to 'change the pace' of this key sentence to emphasize the potential effects on both preference and performance encapsulated by this hypothesis. A simple way to do this would be: "...early herbivory would (i) reduce oviposition..., and also (ii) reduce the performance ...
Lines 85-86. As the last sentence of the introduction is often one of the most 'visited' by readers, I feel this one could deliver a 'self-standing', stronger message. For instance, instead of 'By addressing the above', one alternative could be 'By addressing the effects of previous herbivory by conspecifics on both preference and performance of subsequent..."
Line 94. I think this initial sentence could be moved down in this paragraph.
Line 107. It is clear from the previous sentence, but it would be helpful to clarify that this plant-level herbivory metric represents the mean frequency of attacked leaves/branch.
Figure 1. These photos are great for illustrating the experimental design and study system! I would just suggest adding more details in the legend.
Line 113. feed on.
Lines 115-116. I wonder if this difference of three weeks between caterpillars being removed (from the herbivory treatment) and moths oviposition reflect the phenology of this species in natural ecosystems. In other words, in light of the biology of BTM, a given box tree in nature could experience two separate groups/generations of caterpillars within three weeks?
Line 118. Just to make it more straightforward: "...washed out from leaves".
Line 120-121. This is an important point that could be briefly mentioned in the last paragraph of the introduction, e.g., "our experimental design allows us to quantify... of plant-mediated".
Lines 126-128. I think that first presenting 'why' (i.e., "to prevent larvae from moving from one potted plant to another") and then 'how' (i.e., "we installed box trees in plastic saucers and interspaced plants and filled saucers with a few centimeters of water").
Line 131. All plants had at least five larvae?
Line 133. It would be helpful to emphasize that this value represents the average weight of larvae from one plant individual.
Line 142. Thanks for such a careful explanation; I appreciate your transparency!
Line 150. Could you please describe in more detail the structure of these models? (i.e., define response variable and predictors).
Line 165. With other three models (∆i < 2 - Models 2, 3, and 4), right?
Line 177. How about finishing this sentence after 'season' and then start a new sentence with 'This time-lagged'?
Line 182-183. Maybe the fact that larval frass was washed out could be a potential explanation?
Lines 201-203. This an interesting explanation! I wonder if host plants in the native range of BTM have even higher toxic alkaloids than box trees.
Lines 206-207. I do not follow this idea, could you please clarify? I feel that 'reduced performance of individuals... have been trait-mediated' requires some further explanation.
Line 219-220. The fact that food limitation can trigger cannibalism in this system is fascinating!
Line 221. To avoid repetition, I suggest replacing the first 'Herbivore' by 'Consumer'.
Line 228. Would it be 'with' or 'within'?
Line 237. Perhaps 'negative interactions... generations' could communicate more clearly the results.
Line 241. Because this idea expands to the next sentence, it would be good to mention their main enemies (e.g., parasitoids, predators).
Line 244. It seems that a verb is missing in this sentence, 'causing more damage' is one option.
Line 247. be investigated
Line 248. dedicated
I hope the authors find these comments helpful. Best wishes, Raul.